Absorption spectra (or sensitivity curves) differ for the three types of cones. (Spectral- energy curves and absorption curves such as these have their y axes in log units because they operate over such a wide range. The up-and-down position of the x-axis is therefore arbitrary and does not represent zero absorption.) The pigments in the three cone types have their peak absorptions at about 430, 530, and 560 nanometers, as shown in the graph to the left; the cones are consequently loosely called "blue", "green", and "red", "loosely" because (1) the names refer to peak sensitivities (which in turn are related to ability to absorb light) rather than to the way the pigments would appear if we were to look at them; (2) monochromatic lights whose wavelengths are 430, 530, and 560 nanometers are not blue, green, and red but violet, blue- green, and yellow-green; and (3) if we were to stimulate cones of just one type, we would see not blue, green, or red but probably violet, green, and yellowish-red instead. However unfortunate the terminology is, it is now widely used, and efforts to change embedded terminology usually fail. To substitute terms such as long, middle, and short would be more correct but would put a burden on those of us not thoroughly familiar with the spectrum. With peak absorption in the green, the rod pigment, rhodopsin, reflects blue and red and therefore looks purple. Because it is present in large enough amounts in our retinas that chemists can extract it and look at it, it long ago came to be called visual purple. Illogical as it is, "visual purple" is named for the appearance of the pigment, whereas the terms for cones, "red", "green", and "blue", refer to their relative sensitivities or abilities to absorb light. Not to realize this can cause great confusion. The three cones show broad sensitivity curves with much overlap, especially the red and the green cones. Light at 600 nanometers will evoke the greatest response from red cones, those peaking at 560 nanometers, but will likely evoke some response, even if weaker, from the other two cone types. Thus the red- sensitive cone does not respond only to long-wavelength, or red, light; it just responds better. The same holds for the other two cones. So far I have been dealing with physical concepts: the nature of light and pigments, the qualities of the pigments that reflect light to our eyes, and the qualities of the rod and cone pigments that translate the incoming light into electrical signals. It is the brain that interprets these initial signals as colors. In conveying some feel for the subject, I find it easiest to outline the elementary facts about color vision at the outset, leaving aside for the moment the three-century history of how these facts were established or how the brain handles color.